The Influence of Media Violence on Youth
Top Cited Papers
- 1 December 2003
- journal article
- Published by SAGE Publications in Psychological Science in the Public Interest
- Vol. 4 (3) , 81-110
- https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2003.pspi_1433.x
Abstract
Research on violent television and films, video games, and music reveals unequivocal evidence that media violence increases the likelihood of aggressive and violent behavior in both immediate and long-term contexts. The effects appear larger for milder than for more severe forms of aggression, but the effects on severe forms of violence are also substantial ( r = .13 to .32) when compared with effects of other violence risk factors or medical effects deemed important by the medical community (e.g., effect of aspirin on heart attacks). The research base is large; diverse in methods, samples, and media genres; and consistent in overall findings. The evidence is clearest within the most extensively researched domain, television and film violence. The growing body of video-game research yields essentially the same conclusions. Short-term exposure increases the likelihood of physically and verbally aggressive behavior, aggressive thoughts, and aggressive emotions. Recent large-scale longitudinal studies provide converging evidence linking frequent exposure to violent media in childhood with aggression later in life, including physical assaults and spouse abuse. Because extremely violent criminal behaviors (e.g., forcible rape, aggravated assault, homicide) are rare, new longitudinal studies with larger samples are needed to estimate accurately how much habitual childhood exposure to media violence increases the risk for extreme violence. Well-supported theory delineates why and when exposure to media violence increases aggression and violence. Media violence produces short-term increases by priming existing aggressive scripts and cognitions, increasing physiological arousal, and triggering an automatic tendency to imitate observed behaviors. Media violence produces long-term effects via several types of learning processes leading to the acquisition of lasting (and automatically accessible) aggressive scripts, interpretational schemas, and aggression-supporting beliefs about social behavior, and by reducing individuals' normal negative emotional responses to violence (i.e., desensitization). Certain characteristics of viewers (e.g., identification with aggressive characters), social environments (e.g., parental influences), and media content (e.g., attractiveness of the perpetrator) can influence the degree to which media violence affects aggression, but there are some inconsistencies in research results. This research also suggests some avenues for preventive intervention (e.g., parental supervision, interpretation, and control of children's media use). However, extant research on moderators suggests that no one is wholly immune to the effects of media violence. Recent surveys reveal an extensive presence of violence in modern media. Furthermore, many children and youth spend an inordinate amount of time consuming violent media. Although it is clear that reducing exposure to media violence will reduce aggression and violence, it is less clear what sorts of interventions will produce a reduction in exposure. The sparse research literature suggests that counterattitudinal and parental-mediation interventions are likely to yield beneficial effects, but that media literacy interventions by themselves are unsuccessful. Though the scientific debate over whether media violence increases aggression and violence is essentially over, several critical tasks remain. Additional laboratory and field studies are needed for a better understanding of underlying psychological processes, which eventually should lead to more effective interventions. Large-scale longitudinal studies would help specify the magnitude of media-violence effects on the most severe types of violence. Meeting the larger societal challenge of providing children and youth with a much healthier media diet may prove to be more difficult and costly, especially if the scientific, news, public policy, and entertainment communities fail to educate the general public about the real risks of media-violence exposure to children and youth.Keywords
This publication has 150 references indexed in Scilit:
- What Do Children Value in Entertainment Programs? A Cross-Cultural InvestigationJournal of Communication, 1999
- Predictors of children's interest in violent television programsJournal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 1997
- How real is the portrayal of aggression in television entertainment programming?Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 1995
- Priming Sex-Role Stereotypic Event Schemas With Rock Music Videos: Effects on Impression Favorability, Trait Inferences, and Recall of a Subsequent Male-Female InteractionBasic and Applied Social Psychology, 1989
- Television-Viewing and Family Communication Style as Predictors of Children's Emotional BehaviorJournal of Children in Contemporary Society, 1986
- The effects of aggressive pornography on beliefs in rape myths: Individual differencesJournal of Research in Personality, 1985
- The effects of mass media exposure on acceptance of violence against women: A field experimentJournal of Research in Personality, 1981
- Effects of timing and justification of witnessed aggression on the observers' punitivenessJournal of Research in Personality, 1979
- The effects of a model's success or failure on subsequent aggressive behaviorJournal of Research in Personality, 1978
- The meaning of observed violence: Real vs. fictional violence and consequent effects on aggression and emotional arousalJournal of Research in Personality, 1975