Dogmas of understanding
- 1 January 1997
- journal article
- research article
- Published by Taylor & Francis in Discourse Processes
- Vol. 23 (3) , 567-598
- https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539709545003
Abstract
Investigators of language understanding have made a number of idealizations in order to study it, but many of these idealizations have turned into dogmas—convictions that are impervious to evidence. Because of these dogmas, investigators have often ignored, dismissed, or ruled out of court common features of everyday language such as indirect meaning, word innovation, phrasal utterances, interjections, listener roles, listener background, specialized lexicons, joint actions by speakers and addressees, disfluencies, changes of mind, gestures, eye gaze, pretense, and quotations. I describe eleven common dogmas of understanding, some evidence against them, and some of the dangers they pose for the study of understanding. Using language is fundamentally social, I argue, and social features appear to influence understanding at many, perhaps most, levels of processing.Keywords
This publication has 48 references indexed in Scilit:
- The temporal structure of spoken language understandingPublished by Elsevier ,2002
- Pronouncing “the” as “thee” to signal problems in speakingCognition, 1997
- Contextual coherence and attention in phoneme monitoringJournal of Memory and Language, 1992
- Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parserLanguage and Cognitive Processes, 1989
- Ambiguity, parsing strategies, and computational modelsLanguage and Cognitive Processes, 1988
- On Defining Communicative IntentionsMind & Language, 1986
- Referring as a collaborative processCognition, 1986
- On not being led up the garden path: the use of context by the psychological syntax processorPublished by Cambridge University Press (CUP) ,1985
- Replies and responsesLanguage in Society, 1976
- On coping with ordered and unordered conjunctive sentences.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971