Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memories in gerbils
- 1 November 1986
- journal article
- research article
- Published by Springer Nature in Journal of Comparative Physiology A
- Vol. 158 (6) , 835-851
- https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01324825
Abstract
The aim of this study is to understand what a rodent (Meriones unguiculatus) learns about the geometrical relations between a goal and nearby visual landmarks and how it uses this information to reach a goal. Gerbils were trained to find sunflower seeds on the floor of a light-tight, black painted room illuminated by a single light bulb hung from the ceiling. The position of the seed on the floor was specified by an array of one or more landmarks. Once training was complete, we recorded where the gerbils searched when landmarks were present but the seed was absent. In such tests, gerbils were confronted either with the array of landmarks to which they were accustomed or with a transformation of this array. Animals searched in the appropriate spot when trained to find seeds placed in a constant direction and at a constant distance from a single cylindrical landmark (Fig. 1). Since gerbils look in one spot and not in a circle centred on the landmark, the direction between landmark and goal must be supplied by cues external to the landmark array. Distance, on the other hand, must be measured with respect to the landmark. Tests in which the size of the landmark was altered from that used in training suggest that distance is not learned solely in terms of the apparent size of the landmark as seen from the goal (Fig. 3). Gerbils can still reach a goal defined by an array of landmarks when the room light is extinguished during their approach (Figs. 4, 5). This ability implies that they have already planned a trajectory to the goal before the room is darkened. In order to compute such a trajectory, their internal representation of landmarks and goal needs to contain information about the distances and bearings between landmarks and goal. For planning trajectories, each landmark of an array can be used separately from the others (Fig. 7). Gerbils trained to a goal specified by an array of several landmarks were tested with one or more of the landmarks removed or with the array expanded. They then searched as though they had computed an independent trajectory for each landmark. For instance, gerbils trained with an array of two landmarks were tested with the distance between two landmarks doubled. The animals then searched for seeds in two positions, which were at the correct distance and in the right direction from each landmark. If an internal representation of an array of landmarks is to be used to plan a trajectory, landmarks seen on the ground must be matched to those held in memory. One way in which gerbils do this is by learning properties of individual landmarks, such as their shape, size or surface markings (Figs. 10, 11, 13). For example, gerbils were able to locate seeds defined by a single relevant landmark while ignoring an irrelevant landmark with different features which was placed randomly with respect to the goal. Several experiments (Figs. 4, 12, 13, 14) suggested that, although landmarks may be used independently for computing trajectories, the process of matching landmarks to the gerbil's representation requires a knowledge of the distances and directionsbetween landmarks. We conclude that a gerbil's representation of its environment is complete in that it stores explicitly or can compute from what it has stored the geometric arrangement of landmarks and goal. We discuss the possibility that its spatial memories consist of a set of vectors describing the distance and direction from the goal to each landmark (Fig. 18) and consider the advantages and disadvantages of such a goal-centred memory.Keywords
This publication has 13 references indexed in Scilit:
- Food Storage by Birds and MammalsPublished by Elsevier ,1985
- Spatial OrientationPublished by Walter de Gruyter GmbH ,1984
- Grating acuity of the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus)Behavioural Brain Research, 1983
- Landmark learning in beesJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 1983
- Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cuesLearning and Motivation, 1981
- Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in ratsLearning and Motivation, 1980
- How do we use visual information to control locomotion?Trends in Neurosciences, 1980
- Ecology of Desert Rodents of the U.S.S.R. (Jerboas and Gerbils)Published by Springer Nature ,1975
- Studies in spatial learning. II. Place learning versus response learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1946
- Studies in spatial learning. I. Orientation and the short-cut.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1946