Task-Dependent Constraints in Motor Control: Pinhole Goggles Make the Head Move Like an Eye
Open Access
- 1 April 2000
- journal article
- Published by Society for Neuroscience in Journal of Neuroscience
- Vol. 20 (7) , 2719-2730
- https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.20-07-02719.2000
Abstract
In the 19th century, Donders observed that only one three-dimensional eye orientation is used for each gaze direction. Listing9s law further specifies that the full set of eye orientation vectors forms a plane, whereas the equivalent Donders9 law for the head, the Fick strategy, specifies a twisted two-dimensional range. Surprisingly, despite considerable research and speculation, the biological reasons for choosing one such range over another remain obscure. In the current study, human subjects performed head-free gaze shifts between visual targets while wearing pinhole goggles. During fixations, the head orientation range still obeyed Donders9 law, but in most subjects, it immediately changed from the twisted Fick-like range to a flattened Listing-like range. Further controls showed that this was not attributable to loss of binocular vision or increased range of head motion, nor was it attributable to blocked peripheral vision; when subjects pointed a helmet-mounted laser toward targets (a task with goggle-like motor demands but normal vision), the head followed Listing9s law even more closely. Donders9 law of the head only broke down (in favor of a “minimum-rotation strategy”) when head motion was dissociated from gaze. These behaviors could not be modeled using current “Donders9 operators” but were readily simulated nonholonomically, i.e., by modulating head velocity commands as a function of position and task. We conclude that the gaze control system uses such velocity rules to shape Donders9 law on a moment-to-moment basis, not primarily to satisfy perceptual or anatomic demands, but rather for motor optimization; the Fick strategy optimizes the role of the head as a platform for eye movement, whereas Listing9s law optimizes rapid control of the eye (or head) as a gaze pointer.Keywords
This publication has 37 references indexed in Scilit:
- How do Motor Systems Deal with the Problems of Controlling Three-Dimensional Rotations?Journal of Motor Behavior, 1995
- Vestibular syndromes in the roll plane: Topographic diagnosis from brainstem to cortexAnnals of Neurology, 1994
- Are the Orientations of the Head and Arm Related during Pointing Movements?Journal of Motor Behavior, 1993
- Listing's law for eye, head and arm movements and their synergistic controlExperimental Brain Research, 1991
- Geometric relations of eye position and velocity vectors during saccadesVision Research, 1990
- Computing three-dimensional eye position quaternions and eye velocity from search coil signalsVision Research, 1990
- Bone Scintigraphy in Patients with Sickle Cell Disease and OsteomyelitisAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1989
- A direct test of Listing's law—I. Human ocular torsion measured in static tertiary positionsVision Research, 1986
- The effect on form perception of change of orientation in the third dimension.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1981
- Kinematics of the EyeJournal of the Optical Society of America, 1957