Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults
Top Cited Papers
- 20 December 2006
- journal article
- research article
- Published by American Medical Association (AMA) in JAMA
- Vol. 296 (23) , 2805-14
- https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.296.23.2805
Abstract
Cognitive training has been shown to improve cognitive abilities in older adults but the effects of cognitive training on everyday function have not been demonstrated. To determine the effects of cognitive training on daily function and durability of training on cognitive abilities. Five-year follow-up of a randomized controlled single-blind trial with 4 treatment groups. A volunteer sample of 2832 persons (mean age, 73.6 years; 26% black), living independently in 6 US cities, was recruited from senior housing, community centers, and hospitals and clinics. The study was conducted between April 1998 and December 2004. Five-year follow-up was completed in 67% of the sample. Ten-session training for memory (verbal episodic memory), reasoning (inductive reasoning), or speed of processing (visual search and identification); 4-session booster training at 11 and 35 months after training in a random sample of those who completed training. Self-reported and performance-based measures of daily function and cognitive abilities. The reasoning group reported significantly less difficulty in the instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) than the control group (effect size, 0.29; 99% confidence interval [CI], 0.03-0.55). Neither speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, -0.002 to 0.51) nor memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, -0.06 to 0.46) had a significant effect on IADL. The booster training for the speed of processing group, but not for the other 2 groups, showed a significant effect on the performance-based functional measure of everyday speed of processing (effect size, 0.30; 99% CI, 0.08-0.52). No booster effects were seen for any of the groups for everyday problem-solving or self-reported difficulty in IADL. Each intervention maintained effects on its specific targeted cognitive ability through 5 years (memory: effect size, 0.23 [99% CI, 0.11-0.35]; reasoning: effect size, 0.26 [99% CI, 0.17-0.35]; speed of processing: effect size, 0.76 [99% CI, 0.62-0.90]). Booster training produced additional improvement with the reasoning intervention for reasoning performance (effect size, 0.28; 99% CI, 0.12-0.43) and the speed of processing intervention for speed of processing performance (effect size, 0.85; 99% CI, 0.61-1.09). Reasoning training resulted in less functional decline in self-reported IADL. Compared with the control group, cognitive training resulted in improved cognitive abilities specific to the abilities trained that continued 5 years after the initiation of the intervention. clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00298558.Keywords
This publication has 33 references indexed in Scilit:
- Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: The Freedom House StudyJournal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2005
- Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderlyNeurology, 2005
- Long-Term Effectiveness of Spaced-Retrieval Memory Training for Older Adults with Probable Alzheimer's DiseaseExperimental Aging Research, 2005
- Speed-of-Processing and Driving Simulator Training Result in Improved Driving PerformanceHuman Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 2003
- Incidence of Alzheimer Disease in a Biracial Urban CommunityArchives of Neurology, 2003
- Effects of Cognitive Training Interventions With Older AdultsJAMA, 2002
- Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal dataStatistics in Medicine, 1997
- Everyday Cognitive Competence in Elderly Persons: Conceptual Issues and Empirical FindingsThe Gerontologist, 1996
- The Risk of Nursing Home Placement and Subsequent Death Among Older AdultsJournal of Gerontology, 1992
- Training the elderly on the ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning.Psychology and Aging, 1986